Can intelligence be boosted by a simple task? For some...

Not Exactly Rocket Science
By Ed Yong
Jun 14, 2011 12:00 AMNov 20, 2019 6:02 AM

Newsletter

Sign up for our email newsletter for the latest science news
 

If you train intensively at long-distance running, you’ll find it easier to climb stairs or ride a bike. The running will boost your aerobic fitness and strengthen your leg muscles, providing benefits that transfer to other activities. Does our brain work in the same way? If we train ourselves on a specific mental task, do we become sharper across the board? There’s a multi-million dollar industry that would like you to believe the answer is yes. Best-selling “brain training” games like Brain Age purport to give your brain a “the workout it needs” through a combination of word puzzles, number problems, Su Doku and more. Unfortunately, there is little good evidence that these games improve anything beyond performance on a specific task. There are exceptions. Susanne Jaeggi from the University of Michigan has found that a simple exercise called an “n-back task” could increase the “fluid intelligence” of elementary and middle-school children (well, some of them; more on this later). It’s the latest of a series of studies showing that practicing at a single task can lead to a broader intellectual boost. Fluid intelligence is a broad concept that includes abilities like abstract reasoning, solving new problems, spotting patterns and drawing inferences, rather than relying on knowledge, skills or experience. The n-back task isn’t meant to train all of these. Instead, it is meant to improve working memory – the ability to hold and manipulate pieces of information in our head. A good working memory is essential for problem-solving and reasoning, so Jaeggi reasoned that training the former would improve the latter, in the same way that building a runner’s fitness improves their cycling. She was right. In 2008, she showed that students who trained on the n-back task did much better on fluid intelligence tests than peers who didn’t receive any training. The more they trained, the better they became. It was an exciting result. Fluid intelligence is thought to be strongly influenced by genetics and unaffected by knowledge and education. The prospect of improving it through a simple task is an enticing one. Now, she has turned her attention to children. She recruited 62 children, aged between seven and ten. While half of them simply learned some basic general knowledge questions, the other half trained with a cheerful computerised n-back task. They saw a stream of images where a target object appeared in one of six locations – say, a frog in a lily pond. They had to press a button if the frog was in the same place as it was two images ago, forcing them to store a continuously updated stream of images in their minds. If the children got better at the task, this gap increased so they had to keep more images in their heads. If they struggled, the gap was shortened. Before and after the training sessions, all the children did two reasoning tests designed to measure their fluid intelligence. At first, the results looked disappointing. On average, the n-back children didn’t become any better at these tests than their peers who studied the knowledge questions. But according to Jaeggi, that’s because some of them didn’t take to the training. When she divided the children according to how much they improved at the n-back task, she saw that those who showed the most progress also improved in fluid intelligence. The others did not. Best of all, these benefits lasted for 3 months after the training. That’s a first for this type of study, although Jaeggi herself says that the effect is “not robust.” Over this time period, all the children showed improvements in their fluid intelligence, “probably [as] a result of the natural course of development”. These results are more qualified than those from the first experiment in 2008. In that one, the difference between the n-back group and the controls was very clear. Here, only some of the children who trained their working memory developed better fluid intelligence as a result. And Philip Ackerman, who studies learning and brain training at the University of Illinois, says, “I am concerned about the small sample, especially after splitting the groups on the basis of their performance improvements.” He has a point – the group that showed big improvements in the n-back training only included 18 children. Robert Sternberg, an intelligence researcher at Oklahoma State University, is more convinced. He says, “This paper adds weight to the evidence that fluid ability is trainable. We had known from the so-called Flynn effect that fluid ability has been increasing at least since the early 1900s. But it was not as clear that fluid ability could be increased within a generation. This study suggests that indeed it can be - that this important aspect of intelligence is not static and fixed, but rather is dynamic and flexible.” Why did some of the children benefit from the training while others did not? Perhaps they were simply uninterested in the task, no matter how colourfully it was dressed up with storks and vampires. In Jaeggi’s earlier study with adults, every volunteer signed up themselves and were “intrinsically motivated to participate and train.” By contrast, the kids in this latest study were signed up by their parents and teachers, and some might only have continued because they were told to do so. It’s also possible that the changing difficulty of the game was frustrating for some of the children. Jaeggi says, “The children who did not benefit from the training found the working memory intervention too effortful and difficult, were easily frustrated, and became disengaged. This makes sense when you think of physical training - if you don't try and really run and just walk instead, you won't improve your cardiovascular fitness.” Indeed, a recent study on IQ testing which found that they reflect motivation as well as intelligence. Jaeggi’s next goal is to find out what makes n-back training effective, whether only specific groups of people will benefit from it, and what conditions lead to the best results. “We are quite at the beginning when it comes to that. We know that training time is an issue (the more training, the better), we know that improvement in training is a factor, but other than that, we don't know much. There is some evidence showing that distributed training is better than massed training. There are hundreds of other issues that wait to be clarified.” For the moment, it seems that the flood of n-back apps that followed from Jaeggi’s 2008 study were premature. The task shows some promise, but it isn’t an all-purpose route to a higher intellect. As ever, things are more complicated than that. Sternberg also wants to see if working memory training can improve performance at “more serious endeavours” like school work or job performance, instead of simply on puzzle-like tests. He doesn’t want the n-back tasks to go the way of IQ test – something that is meant to predict a person’s performance, but has become a criterion in itself. “Parents and even schools sometimes seem more concerned with test scores than with the achievement they are supposed to predict or to measure,” he says. In the case of the n-back studies, it would be a shame if people took their scores to mean an automatic intelligence boost. Finally, Sternberg says, “Fluid ability, although important, is only part of intellectual abilities broadly defined. Other abilities, such as creative thinking, practical thinking, and wisdom-based thinking, are at least as important. In our world, we seem more to lack wisdom and ethics as applied to everyday situations than we do the abstract-thinking skills measured by fluid-ability tests.” Reference: Jaeggi, Buschkuehl, Jonides & Shah. 2011. Short- and long-term benefits of cognitive training. For more on the new study, see Jonah Lehrer’s takeMore on brain-training and IQ:

1 free article left
Want More? Get unlimited access for as low as $1.99/month

Already a subscriber?

Register or Log In

1 free articleSubscribe
Discover Magazine Logo
Want more?

Keep reading for as low as $1.99!

Subscribe

Already a subscriber?

Register or Log In

More From Discover
Recommendations From Our Store
Stay Curious
Join
Our List

Sign up for our weekly science updates.

 
Subscribe
To The Magazine

Save up to 40% off the cover price when you subscribe to Discover magazine.

Copyright © 2024 LabX Media Group