Why Hand Gestures Are Important

Waving your hands around may increase your language abilities in some ways

By Joshua Rapp Learn
Mar 5, 2022 3:30 PM
gesture
(Credit:Krakenimages.com/Shutterstock)

Newsletter

Sign up for our email newsletter for the latest science news
 

Our hands have always been able to talk. Even without the highly developed nuance of sign languages, the right gesture can invite a lover, repel an enemy and express confusion or excitement. Aside from many universally understood gestures like the middle finger, we also use our hands in vaguer ways to express more complicated concepts while speaking. But how do these gestures affect our speech and communication in general?

“In development, it is well known that gestures precede speech in many ways, and also predict speech,” says Pilar Prieto, a linguist at the University of Pompeu Fabra in Barcelona. She and her colleagues have been conducting several experiments on the use of gestures to determine their importance in communication, as well as their development in children in recent years.

Arrested Expression

In one study recently published in Speech Communication, Prieto and her colleagues took 20 Italian female volunteers in their 20s and asked them to describe a comic strip. In one experiment, they were asked to sit on their hands so they couldn’t make gestures. In another control, they had no restrictions on their arms.

Prieto and her colleagues recorded the interviews, then analyzed their speech —  counting the number of words they used, the speed at which they spoke, the length of their descriptions, pauses, self-corrections, repetitions, insertions or interruptions and the tone and intensity of their voices.

They found that for the participants who use minimal hand gestures when speaking to begin with, losing the ability to make hand gestures didn’t affect their fluency, acoustic characteristics or production of their speech. But once those participants were eliminated, the researchers found that those who used a lot of hand gestures when normally talking spoke differently without the use of their arms.

“You are less fluent in your speech, also you speak less, and with less creativity,” says Prieto.

In another study, researchers did just the opposite. They encouraged 20 native Italian speakers to narrate a story, encouraging them to use gestures in one condition and not encouraging them in another. In this experiment, they found that the people encouraged to gesture used a larger variety of words in their narratives and were also livelier in their speech. 

Developmental Gestures

In this study from Frontiers in Psychology, Prieto and her colleagues looked at the root of non-referential gestures, which hadn’t been studied as much as referential gestures.

Referential gestures are the kind used when indicating something specific — say, when you make a shape of a ball while describing it or using your hand to indicate how much snow was on the ground. Non-referential gestures are more like body language, and don’t necessarily refer t a specific thing.

But the researchers wanted to better understand how this worked, especially with non-referential gestures. Prieto’s colleague Ingrid Vilà-Giménez, currently at the University of Girona in Spain, conducted earlier research on analyzing whether they could help the linguistic abilities of children develop if they began training the children to use gestures at early ages. Researchers found that learning gestures could help boost language development and storytelling. But they wanted more evidence about why this might be happening.

The researchers conducted a similar experiment as with the adults, giving five- and six-year-olds acomic strip and asking them to tell a story about what was happening in the images. The researchers then repeated the exercise with the same children two years later, recording the videos and analyzing various components of their speech patterns and gestures.

The team found that by five years old, children are already using non-referential gestures in their speech. By the time they were two years older, they used these gestures a lot more in their descriptions. They also produced non-referential gestures more than referential gestures — similar to adults, who barely produce referential gestures while speaking.

Júlia Florit-Pons, a Ph.D. student at the University of Pompeu Fabra, says these gestures were used to help move the discourse forward and to indicate they understand the listener is on the same page as them.

“We believe that these abilities somehow related to more cognitive or pragmatic abilities,” Florit-Pons says, adding that the cognitive ability indicating you know the other person understands you is something that grows with age.

1 free article left
Want More? Get unlimited access for as low as $1.99/month

Already a subscriber?

Register or Log In

1 free articleSubscribe
Discover Magazine Logo
Want more?

Keep reading for as low as $1.99!

Subscribe

Already a subscriber?

Register or Log In

More From Discover
Recommendations From Our Store
Stay Curious
Join
Our List

Sign up for our weekly science updates.

 
Subscribe
To The Magazine

Save up to 40% off the cover price when you subscribe to Discover magazine.

Copyright © 2024 LabX Media Group