Happy New Year! For a limited time only, access all online articles for free.

Is the Internet Rotting Our Brains? No.

The mind is built to reach outside itself and make the world, including our machines, an extension of itself, contends Carl Zimmer.

By Carl Zimmer
Jan 1, 1970 12:00 AMNov 12, 2019 5:19 AM
brain-phone.jpg
Sara Schnadt’s Network captures a sense of the constantly expanding web. | file404 / Shutterstock

Newsletter

Sign up for our email newsletter for the latest science news
 

Our minds are under attack. At least that’s what I keep hearing these days. Thumbing away at our text messages, we are becoming illiterate. (Or is that illiter8?) Blogs make us coarse, YouTube makes us shallow. The internet is damaging our brains, robbing us of our deep thoughts and our ability to remember.

I have a hard time taking these Cassandras of the Computer Age seriously. For one thing, they are much more interested in our fears than in the facts. In his book Txtng: The Gr8 Db8, the British linguist David Crystal demonstrates that many of the dire warnings about texting are little more than urban legends. Texting doesn’t lead to bad spelling, he finds. In fact, Crystal writes, “texting actually improves your literacy, as it gives you more practice in reading and writing.”

More significantly, the ominous warnings feed on a popular misconception about how the mind works. We tend to think of the mind as separated from the world; we imagine information trickling into our senses and reaching our isolated minds, which then turn that information into a detailed picture of reality. The internet and iPhones seem to be crashing the gate of the mind, taking over its natural work and leaving it to wither away. As plausible as this picture may seem, it does a bad job of explaining a lot of recent scientific research. Actually, the mind appears to be adapted for reaching out from our head and making the world, including our machines, an extension of itself.

This concept of the extended mind was first raised in 1998 by two philosophers, Andy Clark, now at the University of Edinburgh, and David Chalmers, now at the Australian National University. In the journal Analysis, they published a short essay in which they asked a simple question: “Where does the mind stop and the rest of the world begin?” Most people might answer, “At the skull.” But Clark and Chalmers set out to convince their readers that the mind is not simply the product of the neurons in the brain. Rather, they argued that the mind is something more: a system made up of the brain plus parts of its environment.

Clark and Chalmers asked their readers to imagine a woman named Inga. Inga hears from a friend that there’s an exhibit at the Museum of Modern Art. She decides to go see it. She recalls that the museum is on 53rd Street and starts walking that way. She accesses her belief that MOMA is on 53rd Street from its storage place in her brain’s memory network. Now imagine a man named Otto, who has Alzheimer’s. His memory is faulty, and so he carries a notebook in which he writes down important details. Like Inga, Otto hears about the exhibit. Since he can’t access the address in his brain, he looks it up in his notebook and then heads off in the same direction.

In the view of Clark and Chalmers, Inga’s brain-based memory and Otto’s notebook are fundamentally the same. Inga’s mind just happens to access information stored away in her brain, while Otto’s mind draws on information stored in his notebook. The notebook, in other words, is part of his extended mind. Clark and Chalmers concluded that real people are actually more like Otto than like Inga: We all have minds that extend out into our environment.

Twelve years later, this argument continues to trigger fierce debate among philosophers, psychologists, and neuroscientists. There is no doubt that the extended mind is a weird concept. One reason it seems so strange is that our minds feel as if they are really totally selfcontained. We innately believe, for example, that as we walk down a street, we are continuously filming a detailed movie of our surroundings and using that mental movie to decide what to do next. But like many beliefs we have about ourselves, this movie is an illusion. Our awareness is, in truth, remarkably narrow.

One of the most spectacular demonstrations of how oblivious we can be was carried out by psychologists Daniel Simons of the University of Illinois and Christopher Chabris at Harvard University. They asked people to watch a video of students passing a basketball on a court. Half the students wore white shirts, the other half black. The subjects had to keep track of how many times the ball was passed by members of one of the teams. In the middle of the game, a gorilla (or rather, a student in a gorilla costume) sauntered through the scene. Many subjects later reported that they never saw the gorilla; their brains discarded it as extraneous.

Inside our head, instead of making a perfect replica of the world, we focus our attention on tiny snippets, using strikingly little information in the process. Dana Ballard, a computer scientist, and psychologist Mary Hayhoe, both of the University of Texas, developed a computer game to measure just how little. They showed subjects a pattern of colored blocks in the upper left-hand corner of the computer monitor. They then had them build a similar pattern of blocks in the lower left-hand corner. To do so, the players used a mouse to grab blocks, one by one, from a collection on the right-hand side of the screen. As the players looked from the original model to the collection of blocks to their own growing pattern, the experimenters tracked their eye movements. They found that players looked at the model at the upper left before they picked up a block, and again afterward. The experiments suggest that in each glance, the players were storing only a single piece of information. The first time they noted a block’s color. The second time they noted its position in the model. Instead of keeping a detailed picture of the blocks in mind, people extracted just tiny scraps of information on a need-to-know basis.

Clark argues that these subjects made the pattern of blocks part of their extended mind. It became a store of knowledge they could dip into, an external repository of information. Memory holds a great deal of data. But the extended mind moves swiftly between outside and inside sources, showing little regard for where information comes from.

Our minds do more than take in information, of course. In the block-building game, for example, some commands go to neurons in the hand in order to move the computer mouse. But our brains don’t make a perfect mental replica of our hands and the mouse and the table. Our hands and eyes constantly send signals to the brain, and that feedback alters the signals coming back out. Hand, eye, and brain are part of the same system.

What is even more remarkable about the brain is that it actually searches for new things to make part of this feedback system. Imagine you are poking a stick into an animal’s burrow. As you poke away, you are aware of what the far end of the stick is touching, not the end you’re holding in your hand. This kind of extended sensation appears to be the result of a reorganization of the brain. Scientists have found that when test monkeys spent five minutes learning how to use a rake, some of the neurons in their hands began behaving in a new way. They began to fire in response to stimuli at the end of the rake, not on the monkey’s hand. Training the monkeys to use the rakes caused these neurons to change—reacting to objects lying within rake’s reach rather than arm’s reach.

The eagerness with which the brain merges with tools has made it possible to create some stunning mind-machine interfaces. For instance, Miguel Nicolelis of Duke University and his colleagues put electrodes in the brains of monkeys to link them to a robot arm. The monkeys quickly learned how to move the arm around with pure thought; their neurons reorganized, establishing a new feedback loop between brain and robot arm.

Humans are proving just as good at this merger of mind and machine. The U.S. Navy has developed a flight suit for helicopter pilots that delivers little puffs of air on the side of the pilot’s body as his helicopter tilts in that direction. The pilot responds to the puffs by tilting away from them, and the suit passes those signals on to the helicopter’s steering controls. Pilots who train with this system can learn to fly blindfolded or to carry out complex maneuvers, such as holding the helicopter in a stationary hover. The helicopter becomes, in effect, part of the pilot’s body, linked back to his or her mind.

Some people may be horrified by how passionately people are taking to their laptops and smart phones. But to Clark it would be surprising if we didn’t. We are, in Clark’s words, “natural-born cyborgs.”

The extended mind theory doesn’t just change the way we think about the mind. It also changes how we judge today’s mind-altering technologies. There’s nothing unnatural about relying on the internet for information. After all, we constantly consult the world around us like a kind of visual Wikipedia. Nor is there anything bad about our brains’ being altered by these new technologies, any more than there is something bad about a monkey’s brain changing as it learns how to play with a rake.

Neuroscientists will soon be able to offer fresh ways to enhance the brain, whether with drugs or with implants. To say that these are immoral because they defile our true selves—our isolated, distinct minds—is to ignore biology. Our minds already extend out into the environment, and the changes we make to the environment already alter our minds.

Socrates worried that writing would make people forgetful and unwise. Sure enough, writing did rob us of some gifts, such as the ability to recite epic poems like The Iliad from memory. But it also created a much larger pool of knowledge from which people could draw, a pool that has continued to expand.

There’s no point in trying to hack apart the connections between the inside and the outside of the mind. Instead we ought to focus on managing and improving those connections. For instance, we need more powerful ways to filter the information we get online so we don’t get a mass case of distractibility. Some people may fear that trying to fine-tune the brain-internet connection is an impossible task. But if we’ve learned anything, it’s not to underestimate the mind’s ability to adapt to the changing world.

Additional reporting by Amber Angelle.

More From Discover
Stay Curious
Join
Our List

Sign up for our weekly science updates.

 
Subscribe
To The Magazine

Save up to 40% off the cover price when you subscribe to Discover magazine.

Copyright © 2025 LabX Media Group